Does binge drinking in teenagers affect their everyday prospective memory?

Words: 94
Pages: 1
Subject: Uncategorized

Effects of drinking patterns on prospective memory performance in college students. Please choose ONE of the articles posted below and write a 2-page reaction paper on it. There are no specific requirements about what to write about but please talk about whatever you find interesting. The paper will be a 2-page Word document using a 12-point font, normal default margins, and double spacing. It must be at least 1.75 pages and not more than 2.25 pages long. Please include a title page with your info and the name of the article, 2 pages of writing, and a bibliography of the paper you picked. Think of this like a ″reaction paper″… I do not want a book report in which you just tell me about the article. I prefer to see your ″reactions″; i.e., what you learned, was it interesting, did it agree/disagree with what we talked about in class, why you picked it, etc. So, start the paper with a brief paragraph about the main findings of the article and then give me your thoughts and opinions on it. You will be graded on how much of the 2 pages is your ideas about the article, the clarity with which you present your ideas, and how well you can relate what you found to class. Below are some suggestions (not requirements) to help with the rest of the paper: why you picked the article and why it is interesting to you insights you learned from the article that you think are important how this article relates to a class topic anything else you think is relevant Authors: Zamroziewicz, Marta. Department of Psychology, Trinity College, Hartford, CT, US Raskin, Sarah A., ORCID 0000-0002-4310-4278 . Department of Psychology, Trinity College, Hartford, CT, US, sarah.raskin@trincoll.edu Tennen, Howard. Department of Community Medicine and Health Care, University of Connecticut School of Medicine, CT, US Austad, Carol S.. Department of Psychology, Central Connecticut State University, CT, US Wood, Rebecca M.. Department of Psychology, Central Connecticut State University, CT, US Fallahi, Carolyn R.. Department of Psychology, Central Connecticut State University, CT, US Dager, Alecia D.. Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Hartford Hospital, Hartford, CT, US Sawyer, Broderick. Department of Neurobiology, Yale University, CT, US Leen, Samantha. Department of Neurobiology, Yale University, CT, US Pearlson, Godfrey D.. Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Hartford Hospital, Hartford, CT, US Address: Raskin, Sarah A., Department of Psychology, Trinity College, Hartford, CT, US, 06106, sarah.raskin@trincoll.edu Source: Neuropsychology, Vol 31(2), Feb, 2017. pp. 191-199. NLM Title Abbreviation: Neuropsychology Publisher: US : American Psychological Association Other Publishers: US : Educational Publishing Foundation US : Philadelphia Clinical Neuropsychology Group United Kingdom : Taylor & Francis ISSN: 0894-4105 (Print) 1931-1559 (Electronic) Language: English Keywords: alcohol, adolescence, prospective memory, episodic memory, binge drinking Abstract: Objective: Traditional college students are at a critical juncture in the development of prospective memory (PM). Their brains are vulnerable to the effects of alcohol. Method: There were 123 third and fourth year college students, 19–23 years old, who completed the Self-Rating Effects of Alcohol (SREA), Modified Timeline Follow-back (TFLB), Brief Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Scale (BYAACS), and Alcohol Effects Questionnaire (AEQ) once per month on a secure online database, as reported elsewhere (Dager et al., 2013). Data from the 6 months immediately before memory testing were averaged. In a single testing session participants were administered the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview–Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders-Fourth Edition-Text Revision (MINI-DSM–IV–TR), measures of PM (event-based and time-based), and retrospective memory (RM). Based on the average score of six consecutive monthly responses to the SREA, TLFB, and AEQ, students were classified as nondrinkers, light drinkers, or heavy drinkers (as defined previously; Dager et al., 2013). Alcohol-induced amnesia (blackout) was measured with the BYAACS. Results: We found a relationship between these alcohol use classifications and time-based PM, such that participants who were classified as heavier drinkers were more likely to forget to perform the time-based PM task. We also found that self-reported alcohol-induced amnesia (blackouts) during the month immediately preceding memory testing was associated with lower performance on the event-based PM task. Participants’ ability to recall the RM tasks suggested the PM items were successfully encoded even when they were not carried out, and we observed no relationship between alcohol use and RM performance. Conclusion: Heavy alcohol use in college students may be related to impairments in PM. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved) Document Type: Journal Article Subjects: *Alcohol Drinking Patterns; *Amnesia; *Episodic Memory; *Prospective Memory; Binge Drinking; College Students Medical Subject Headings (MeSH): Alcohol Drinking; Alcoholic Intoxication; Binge Drinking; Female; Humans; Intention; Male; Memory, Episodic; Retrospective Studies; Students; Young Adult PsycInfo Classification: Drug & Alcohol Usage (Legal) (2990) Substance Abuse & Addiction (3233) Population: Human Male Female Location: US Age Group: Adulthood (18 yrs & older) Young Adulthood (18-29 yrs) Tests & Measures: Modified Timeline Follow-back Self-Rating Effects of Alcohol Brief Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Scale Java Neuropsychological Test Memory for Intentions Test Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview DOI: 10.1037/t18597-000 Alcohol Effects Questionnaire DOI: 10.1037/t01803-000 Grant Sponsorship: Sponsor: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, US Grant Number: RO1 AA016599 Recipients: Pearlson, Godfrey D. Methodology: Empirical Study; Quantitative Study Format Covered: Electronic Publication Type: Journal; Peer Reviewed Journal Publication History: First Posted: Nov 14, 2016; Accepted: Jul 31, 2016; Revised: Jul 27, 2016; First Submitted: Feb 21, 2014 Release Date: 20201026 Correction Date: 20201026 Copyright: American Psychological Association. 2016 Digital Object Identifier: http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.ccclib.nocccd.edu/10.1037/neu0000313 PMID: 27841457 PsycARTICLES Identifier: neu-31-2-191 Accession Number: 2016-55069-001 Number of Citations in Source: 72 Translate Full Text: Choose Language Effects of Drinking Patterns on Prospective Memory Performance in College Students This content may contain URLs/links that would redirect you to a non-EBSCO site. EBSCO does not endorse the accuracy or accessibility of these sites, nor of the content therein. ✖ Contents Method Participants Measures Analyses Results Demographic Differences Among Drinking Groups PM and RM Performance on Time-Based and Event-Based Tasks Influence of Sex on PM and RM Performance Drinking Patterns and PM and RM Performance Influence of Alcohol Incidents (Blackouts) Discussion Implications References Full Text Listen By: Marta Zamroziewicz Department of Psychology and Neuroscience Program, Trinity College Sarah A. Raskin Department of Psychology and Neuroscience Program, Trinity College; Howard Tennen Department of Community Medicine and Health Care, University of Connecticut School of Medicine Carol S. Austad Department of Psychology, Central Connecticut State University Rebecca M. Wood Department of Psychology, Central Connecticut State University Carolyn R. Fallahi Department of Psychology, Central Connecticut State University Alecia D. Dager Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Hartford Hospital, Hartford, Connecticut, and Department of Psychiatry, Yale University Broderick Sawyer Department of Neurobiology, Yale University Samantha Leen Department of Neurobiology, Yale University Godfrey D. Pearlson Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Hartford Hospital, and Department of Psychiatry and Department of Neurobiology, Yale University Acknowledgement: Marta Zamroziewicz is now at the Department of Neuroscience, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. Broderick Sawyer is now at the Department of Psychology, University of Louisville. This study was funded in part by a grant from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism RO1 AA016599 to Godfrey D. Pearlson. Sarah A. Raskin has a financial disclosure as the author of the Memory for Intentions Test. We thank Gayna Swart for aiding with statistical analyses. Prospective memory (PM) is a form of episodic memory, and involves the ability to remember to carry out an intention at some future point in time (Brandimonte, Einstein, & McDaniel, 1996; Kvavilashvili & Ellis, 1996). PM is vital in daily life for functions such as taking medications, turning off the stove, completing assignments, and maintaining appointments, and it is more highly correlated with performance on daily tasks than traditional memory measures (Wilson, 1987). Recent studies have suggested that PM may be affected by heavy alcohol use (e.g., Weinborn et al., 2013). The current study examined PM performance in college students as a function of alcohol use. PM may require time-cued remembering (e.g., remembering to return a phone call at 3:00 p.m.; Levy & Loftus, 1984; Wilkens & Baddeley, 1988), or may be prompted by an event-cue (e.g., remembering to take a roast out of the oven in response to the oven timer; Einstein & McDaniel, 1990; Harris & Wilkens, 1982; Kvavilashvili, 1992). McDaniel and Einstein’s (2000) multiprocess theory posits that the strategic encoding, monitoring, and retrieval demands of a given PM task will likely vary by these characteristics of the target cue. Thus, in most studies, event-based tasks have been found to be easier for individuals to perform, most likely because time-based tasks require the person to perform more self-initiated monitoring and retrieval to bring the intention to mind and check a clock or watch (Glisky, 1996; Park, Hertzog, Kidder, Morrell, & Mayhorn, 1997; Sellen, Louie, Harris, & Wilkins, 1997). PM is hypothesized to place more demands on self-initiated monitoring and retrieval processes as compared to retrospective memory (RM; e.g., McDaniel & Einstein, 2000). In fact, PM is dissociable from RM at the neural (e.g., Simons, Scholvinck, Gilbert, Frith, & Burgess, 2006), cognitive (e.g., Salthouse, Berish, & Siedlecki, 2004), and functional (e.g., Woods et al., 2008) levels. PM is presumed to encompass a variety of cognitive processes (e.g., Smith & Bayen, 2004). This includes the formation of the intention, strategic monitoring during the retention interval, recognition of the external cue, and an effortful and controlled search for retrospective recall, otherwise referred to as self-initiated retrieval. Finally, the actual recall and execution of the intention occurs and the PM task is (or is not) completed successfully. Thus, all measures of PM have a delay between the encoding and retrieval of the prospective task; there must be no explicit prompt when the occasion to act occurs; and there must be a separate ongoing activity (e.g., Einstein & McDaniel, 1990). Imaging studies have suggested that PM depends on rostral prefrontal cortex (rPFC) functioning, and, in particular, Brodmann area 10 (Benoit et al., 2011). rPFC has been shown to be engaged during the delay period that occurs between the intention formation and the execution of the intention (Burgess, Gonen-Yaacovi, & Volle, 2011). Brain activation during PM tasks has been distinguished from activation in areas associated with vigilance, dual task performance, and working memory (West, 2008). Not surprisingly, damage to prefrontal regions can significantly impair PM functioning (Burgess, Alderman, Volle, Benoit, & Gilbert, 2009; Crews, He, & Hodge, 2007; Okuda et al., 1998; Sowell, Delis, Stiles, & Jernigan, 2001), and PM impairments have been measured in neurological disorders that are presumed to include dysfunction of prefrontal structures (Carey et al., 2006; Raskin, Buckheit, & Waxman, 2012; Raskin et al., 2014; Raskin et al., 2011). In addition, these frontal structures and associated connections continue to develop past adolescence and may be especially vulnerable to the neurotoxic effects of alcohol during this time (Jacobus & Tapert, 2013), which may put college aged students at particular risk (Dager et al., 2013). Adolescents with alcohol use disorder (AUD) demonstrate smaller prefrontal cortex volumes (Medina et al., 2008; Thomasius et al., 2003) and binge drinking in adolescence has been associated with increased prefrontal and parietal blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) response but decreased hippocampal BOLD response during verbal learning, perhaps reflecting overengagement of task-related frontoparietal systems to compensate for deficient medial temporal involvement (Schweinsburg, McQueeny, Nagel, Eyler, & Tapert, 2010). There is evidence that the adolescent hippocampus is also particularly vulnerable to heavy drinking (e.g., Welch, Carson, & Lawrie, 2013), and alcohol effects on the hippocampus could also contribute to retrospective aspects of PM failures. In particular, it has been suggested that alcohol blackouts may be related to alcohol effects on the hippocampus. To our knowledge, the relationship between alcohol-induced amnesia (blackouts) and PM has not been investigated in adolescents or emerging adults, yet blackouts are reported to occur commonly (White, Signer, Kraus, & Swartzwelder, 2004). Blackouts do not necessarily follow binge drinking although they are associated with a sudden rise in blood alcohol level (Ziegler et al., 2005), an effect that has also been shown to disrupt frontal lobe-mediated memory functions (Weissenborn & Duka, 2003) and hippocampal ones (Welch et al., 2013). In developmental studies, emerging adults (ages 17–20) tend to outperform adolescents (ages 13–16) on PM tasks (Wang, Kliegel, Yang, & Liu, 2006; Ward, Shum, McKinlay, Baker-Tweney, & Wallace, 2005). In particular, tasks high in self-initiated processing and low in environmental support are especially challenging for adolescents (in this study, ages 11–14) (Wang et al., 2011). This improvement in PM efficiency may be related to the development of controlled behavior in general. Thus, any factors that may influence the maturation of brain functions, such as alcohol consumption, could affect the development of PM in this period of life (Wang et al., 2011). Alcohol consumption, in fact, is not trivial in this age group. Individuals between the ages of 12 and 20 account for 11% of all alcohol consumed in the United States, with 90% of alcohol consumption in this age group being characterized as heavy drinking (Centers for Disease Control, 2010). Binge drinking (i.e., five or more drinks for men and four or more drinks for women in a single drinking episode; National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 2004) is especially prevalent in college-aged students, with more than 44% of these individuals binge drinking every 2 weeks and more than 19% of these individuals binge drinking more than three times per week (Chen, Dufour, & Yi, 2004). The relationship between PM performance and alcohol use has been studied most often in adults. Generally, adults with substance use problems report more frequent PM complaints, both in self-report and naturalistic PM daily tasks (Weinborn et al., 2013). In one study heavy alcohol users reported 31.2% more problems with long-term PM than nondrinkers and 23.7% more problems than individuals who report drinking small amounts of alcohol (Ling et al., 2003). Heavy drinkers with a diagnosis of alcohol dependence have been found to perform more poorly on measures of event-based PM when compared to social drinkers (Griffiths et al., 2012), and time-based PM when compared with matched control participants (Platt, Kamboj, Italiano, Rendell, & Curran, 2016). Binge drinkers have been reported to have deficits in time-based PM on the Cambridge Prospective Memory Test (CAMPROMT; Heffernan & O’Neill, 2012). In studies of younger subjects, self-report findings have been mixed. One recent study found that short-term binge drinking participants ages 17–19 did not self-report more PM lapses than those who did not binge drink (Heffernan, Clark, Bartholomew, Ling, & Stephens, 2010), but a previous study of binge drinking participants ages 16–19 found that they did (Heffernan & Bartholomew, 2006). In a group of emerging adults (mean age 18.7) who were chronic alcohol users and whose alcohol consumption was long-term and heavy, as opposed to the short-term binge drinkers, global impairments in PM were also reported (Heffernan et al., 2006). In studies that have used a standardized clinical measure of PM (Memory for Intentions Screening Test; Raskin, 2009), rather than self-report, substance use was associated with poorer performance in adults (Weinborn, Woods, O’Toole, Kellogg, & Moyle, 2011) and emerging adults (ages 16–18; Winward, Hanson, Bekman, Tapert, & Brown, 2014). In particular, participants who reported higher levels of alcohol consumption had more difficulties with the time-based tasks and made more omission errors as well as task substitutions (Weinborn et al., 2011). Performance on the time-based items of the MIST also predicted risk-taking behaviors in both adults with substance use disorder and college-aged drinkers (Weinborn et al., 2013). Although studies have examined how excessive alcohol consumption in college-aged students affects PM performance, more examination of this issue is necessary. Most studies have relied upon self-reports of PM, the accuracy of which is not known. Additionally, there are limited comparisons of PM between different cohorts, including nondrinkers, low to moderate drinkers, and heavy drinkers, in studies using college-aged samples. The primary aim of this study is to determine the effects of drinking behavior on PM functioning in college students. More specifically, the aims are to determine if heavy drinking, including binge drinking, has an effect on either time- or event-based PM and to determine if frequency of blackouts has a relationship with time- or event-based PM. Method Participants Participants were 123 third and fourth year undergraduate college students between the ages of 19 and 23 years (M = 20.42 ± 0.92 years). All attended a small liberal arts college, and were originally recruited to participate in a larger NIAAA-funded study BARCS (Brain and Alcohol Research in College Students; Dager et al., 2013). Initial recruiting was accomplished via school email, flyers, and classroom visits. Exclusion criteria included history of central neurological disorders, head injury accompanied by loss of consciousness of over 1 hr, or concussion within 30 days of participation. Each participant was individually consented with Institute Review Board (IRB) approved consent forms and assigned a randomly generated ID number to protect their identity. Demographic information collected from participants is presented in Table 1. neu-31-2-191-tbl1a.gifDemographic Information by Group Membership Measures Alcohol use assessment BARCS participants received via email a link to a series of secure monthly online questionnaires. Included in these were the Modified Timeline Follow-back (TFLB; Sobell, Maisto, Sobell, & Cooper, 1979), Self-Rating Effects of Alcohol (SREA; Schuckit, Tiff, Smith, Wies-Beck, & Kalmtin, 1997), Brief Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Scale (BYAAS; Kahler, Hustad, Barnett, Strong, & Borsari, 2008), and Alcohol Effects Questionnaire (AEQ; Rohsenow, 1983). For the current study, data were averaged for each questionnaire completed monthly during the 6 months immediately preceding memory testing. The primary variables of interest acquired from these measures were frequency of alcohol consumption, frequency of binge drinking, the number of times the person experienced an alcohol-related blackout (using the scale from the BYAACS as follows: 1 = never; 2 = 1–2 times; 3 = 3–5 times; 4 = more than 5 times), and the maximum number of drinks consumed in one sitting. Current and past Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders-Fourth Edition–Text Revised (DSM–IV–TR) (American Psychiatric Association, 1991) diagnoses for psychotic, anxiety, mood, and substance use disorders were ascertained using the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI; Sheehan et al., 1998). Data on frequencies of diagnoses from the MINI are presented in Table 2. Data on use of other substances is presented in Table 3. neu-31-2-191-tbl2a.gifFrequencies of Diagnoses From the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) neu-31-2-191-tbl3a.gifNumber of Days of Use of Other Substances in the Past Month (1 = Never; 2 = 1–2; 3 = 3–5; 4 = 6–9; 5 = 10–14; 6 = 20 or More) PM assessment PM tasks were embedded within the BARCS testing session. The time-based measure occurred during the self-assessment alcohol use online survey of the BARCS testing session. At the start of the survey, participants were asked to record the current survey question on a colored sheet of paper in their testing packet after 15 min of working on the survey. To establish salience for this task, participants were told that the experimenter was tracking the timing of the survey to ensure that it was not excessive in length. The ongoing task was determined to be a sufficiently distracting as it sought detailed information about life stress, mood, as well as alcohol and drug use. Thus, the time-based PM task was to record the current survey question, the time delay was 15 min, and the ongoing task was a self-assessment online survey. The event-based measure was also administered during the BARCS testing session, in this case during the computer-administered Java Neuropsychological Test (JANET) ([http://janet.glahngroup.org]). At the start of the computerized task, participants were instructed to hand the “cash voucher” located in their testing packet to the experimenter as soon as they had completed the computerized task. Salience was attached to this task by having the experimenter explain to participants that to be compensated for their participation in the study, they needed to turn in the “cash voucher.” The ongoing task, which measured perceptual motor speed, incidental learning, executive function, and impulsivity, was determined to be a sufficiently demanding ongoing task. Thus, the event-based PM task was to hand the cash voucher to the experimenter, the time delay was the amount of time that it took the participant to complete the JANET task (typically 10 to 15 min) and the ongoing task was the JANET, a computerized test of cognition. Items were scored as either zero or one. For the time-based task, no implementation of the PM task, incorrect implementation of the task, or correct implementation of the task at the incorrect time was scored as zero and the correct implementation of the PM task was scored as one. For the event-based task, no implementation of the PM task or incorrect implementation of the task was scored as zero and correct implementation of the task in response to the appropriate cue was scored as one. Although neither PM task has been used in previous studies, both are modeled after the types of tasks that are in the Memory for Intentions Test (MIST; Raskin et al., 2011). As this was part of a larger study (BARCS) it was not possible to include a test the length of the MIST. A series of pilot studies were performed to create tasks at a level of difficulty that avoided either ceiling or floor effects, though no formal validation study was undertaken. Retrospective recognition memory assessment Participants were also asked to complete a brief retrospective recognition questionnaire. Through two multiple-choice questions, participants were asked to identify the correct PM tasks that they had been instructed to complete. These were included to be sure that participants had understood and encoded the task instructions successfully. Correct responses were scored as one and incorrect responses were scored as zero. Defining alcohol groups Participants were divided into three alcohol consumption categories based on responses to the TLFB, AEQ, and SREA and the AUD diagnosis based on published criteria (Dager et al., 2013). Nondrinkers were those who reported they had never consumed alcohol. Light drinkers (a) did not meet current or past criteria for an AUD and (b) drank ˂50% of the weeks during the preceding 6 months as determined from the average of surveys received during the 6 months immediately preceding the memory testing session. Heavy drinkers either (a) met criteria for a current AUD or (b) drank ≥50% of the weeks in the preceding 6 months and binge drank for more than half of the number of drinking incidents reported. Desсrіptive findings related to the drinking behavior of each of these groups are presented in Table 4 along with a series of one-way analyses of variances (ANOVAs) with Tukey honest significant difference (HSD) post hoc mean difference scores that demonstrate the differences between the groups on self-report measures of drinking behavior. A scatterplot of the blackout data is presented in Figure 1. neu-31-2-191-tbl4a.gifMeans (SDs) and Ranges, Overall One-Way ANOVAs, Pair-Wise Comparisons (Tukey HSD), and Effect Sizes for Drinking Behavior of the Three Groups neu-31-2-191-fig1a.gifFigure 1. Number of individuals with correct responses on the event-based task by blackouts. 1 Blackouts 1 = never; 2 = 1–2 times; 3 = 3–4 times; 4 = 5 or more. Analyses Chi-square goodness-of-fit tests were used to compare the groups on the prospective memory and retrospective memory measures. A correlational analysis was used to examine the relationship between number of blackouts in the past month and PM performance. Results Demographic Differences Among Drinking Groups A χ2 goodness-of-fit test revealed significant differences in gender distribution across groups (χ2(2) = 6.65, p = .036). Post hoc Bonferroni confidence intervals (family α level = 0.05) indicated that significantly fewer women (20%) than men (40%) were categorized as heavy drinkers (see Table 1). There were no significant differences in ethnic distribution, racial distribution, or age across groups. PM and RM Performance on Time-Based and Event-Based Tasks Chi-square goodness-of-fit tests revealed that participants performed significantly better overall on the time-based than event-based tasks of PM (χ2(1) = 17.472, p ˂ .01). Performance on RM tasks did not differ between time-based and event-based cues (χ2(1) = 0.23, p ˃ .05). Influence of Sex on PM and RM Performance There was no sex performance difference either on the PM time-based task (male M = 0.74, SD = 0.44; female M = 0.75, SD = 0.44), F(1, 121) = .023, p = .88 or on the PM event-based task (male M = 0.50, SD = 0.06; female M = 0.49, SD = 0.50), F(1, 121) = .009, p = .92. Drinking Patterns and PM and RM Performance A χ2 test of Independence was conducted to evaluate the difference among the three drinking groups on their performance on the PM tasks (time-based, event-based). The between-groups difference was significant, χ2(2, N = 123) = 12.06, p ˂ .01 for the time-based tasks with a moderate effect size (d = 0.65), but not for the event-based tasks (χ2(2, N = 123) = 0.58, ns; see Table 5). neu-31-2-191-tbl5a.gifPerformance of the Three Groups on the Prospective Memory (PM) Tasks Follow-up pairwise comparisons were conducted for the time-based task to determine which groups differed significantly from each other, using the Bonferroni approach to control for Type 1 error (overall Family α = .05). Compared with heavy drinkers a higher proportion of nondrinkers scored correctly on the time-based PM task. Comparisons of performance on time-based events by drinking behavior reveals nondrinkers had a higher proportion of correct responses than heavy drinkers (see Table 5). Table 6 provides the χ2 and significance values. neu-31-2-191-tbl6a.gifChi-Square and p-Values for the Comparisons Between Groups on the Time-Based Prospective Memory (PM) Task There were no significant differences between the three groups on either of the RM tasks (time: χ2(2, N = 123) = 5.16, p = .08; event: χ2(2, N = 123) = 2.10, p = .35; see Table 7). neu-31-2-191-tbl7a.gifPerformance of the Groups on the Retrospective Memory (RM) Tasks Influence of Alcohol Incidents (Blackouts) Pearson bivariate correlation revealed a significant relationship between number of blackouts in the preceding month and performance on the event-based PM task (r(112) = 0.21, p ˂ .05), but not the time-based PM task (r(112) = 0.13, p ˃ .05). Figure 1 shows the relationship between numbers of blackouts and score on the event-based task. There was no significant relationship between either of the RM tasks and blackouts (time: r(112) = 0.10, p = .92 and event: r(112) = 0.25, p = .81). Discussion This study aimed to compare PM performance among college-aged individuals with different drinking patterns. Although previous research has examined PM and alcohol consumption in this population, most PM measures have been self-report and have not compared different drinking patterns. The PM performance measures implemented in the present study included both a time-based and an event-based task. Overall, participants performed better on the time-based than the event-based PM tasks. Different indicators of drinking behavior had differential effects on PM performance. Heavy drinking was specifically related to the greatest impairment in time-based PM performance. Blackouts were specifically related to event-based PM performance. An unexpected finding was that participants overall performed better on the time- than the event-based PM tasks. Most previous examinations of performance-based PM have reported better performance on event-based than time-based tasks (Raskin, 2009). In fact, surprisingly, alcohol consumption has been linked to superior performance on an event-based PM task without effects on a time-based PM task (Arana et al., 2011). The difficulty of time-based tasks may reflect that participants are expected to self-initiate a response at a specific time in the absence of other cues (Park et al., 1997). Presumably, the healthy young adult sample in the current study did not have difficulty with time monitoring during the time-based task. It is possible that, given their tight schedules as college students, they are in the habit of monitoring time. More likely, because the two PM measures utilized different ongoing tasks, the attention demands of the ongoing task during the event-based trial may have made it particularly challenging. The ongoing task during the event-based trial was a series of cognitive assessments that, by their nature, require considerable attention. The ongoing time-based task was a self-report survey from which it may have been easier to disengage. RM scores were consistently high, indicating that the PM measures in this study were properly encoded, and that the appropriate degree of PM “intentionality” was achieved (Burgess, Quayle, & Frith, 2001). The processes underlying this retrospective component of PM are not fully developed in adolescence, but performance falls in older adulthood (Zollig et al., 2007). Thus, the high level of retrospective performance by the emerging adults in the present study suggests that this age group may be more efficient in the retrospective aspect of PM than the prospective aspect. However, these tasks were quite simple with only a single RM task to be recalled at one time. Thus, tasks with a greater RM load may prove more challenging for this age group. Drinking behavior was related to time- but not event-based PM. Specifically, drinking was related to impaired time-based PM performance and impairment was greater for heavier drinkers in a dose-dependent manner. This is consistent with previous findings using the CAMPROMT (Heffernan & O’Neill, 2012) and the MIST (Weinborn et al., 2013; Winward et al., 2014). Because the time-based tasks may be more heavily dependent on PFC structures, it may be that they are more sensitive to alcohol effects. These impairments have also been related to inefficient self-initiation, which may result in difficulties with detection of appropriate cues (Griffiths et al., 2012). This suggests that PM may be a useful avenue to explore in attempts to understand the relationship among impulsivity and initiation of alcohol abuse. In general, our findings are consistent with other studies that have demonstrated that social drinkers tend to be less impaired than heavy drinkers on PM measures. Alcohol users who consume excessively show more errors on long-term, short-term, and internally cued PM on the PM Questionnaire (PMQ), a self-report measure of PM (Heffernan & Bartholomew, 2006) and heavy users have been found to be 30% more likely to report compromised PM abilities (Arana et al., 2011; Ling et al., 2003). Beyond this dose-dependent effect, chronic alcoholism appears to be associated with greater cognitive impairment than sporadic heavy alcohol consumption (Brown, Tapert, Granholm, & Delis, 2000) although irregular alcohol consumption can be enough to provoke some degree of cognitive impairment (Sanhueza, Garciá-Morena, & Expósito, 2011). We also found that alcohol-related blackouts were related to event- but not to time-based PM. Blackouts are associated with a rapid rise in blood alcohol and some researchers have theorized that they are because of impaired memory consolidation (Rose & Grant, 2010). It has long been presumed that PM requires adequate hippocampal activity in addition to that of PFC (Poppenk, Moscovitch, McIntosh, Ozcelik, & Craik, 2010) but that the role of the hippocampus is in remembering the content of the task to be remembered whereas PFC is involved in remembering to remember (e.g., Umeda, Nagumo, & Kato, 2006). Because event-based tasks have more content to be remembered (i.e., both the cue and the intention) it is possible that reduced hippocampal functioning could selectively affect these items. Numerous studies have uncovered deficits in time- but not event-based PM. To our knowledge no prior study has yielded a dissociation in which varied aspects of alcohol use affect time-based PM and event-based PM differentially. There is some evidence to suggest that time and event-based PM are mediated by separate brain networks (Okuda et al., 2007). For example, in one study while both event- and time-based PM induced activation in the posterior frontal and parietal cortices, and deactivation in the medial rostral prefrontal cortex, there was activation specific to each condition (Gonneaud et al., 2014). Occipital areas were more activated during event-based PM, while a network comprising the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the cuneus/precuneus and, to a lesser extent, the inferior parietal lobule, superior temporal gyrus, and the cerebellum, was more activated in time-based PM. Zollig et al. (2007) suggested that occipital activation in event-based tasks reflected target checking or cue detection while the regions activated in the time-based tasks reflected time-estimation and monitoring. Implications The present study is limited by the nature of the PM measures. It is likely that the ongoing tasks were not of equal difficulty and comparisons between the tasks are difficult to interpret. In addition, only simple performance measures were used and these tasks were limited to a single time-based item and a single event-based item, both of which were binary, limiting the range of data collected. Future studies might investigate this question with standardized measures like the MIST, laboratory measures or self-report measures in conjunction with each other. The present study found that alcohol consumption patterns differentially affected time-based PM performance in a sample of college students. A linear model of alcohol consumption and PM performance in this age group developed by Arana and colleagues (2011) revealed that while the first and second predictors of PM performance were the self-reported quantity of alcohol use, the third predictor was number of years since first alcohol use. Additionally, a link between hippocampal volume and age of first alcohol use has been reported (Casey & Jones, 2010). Certainly, lifetime history of alcohol use needs to be taken into consideration when extrapolating on cognitive repercussions of alcohol consumption. Future work implementing both performance and self-report measures of PM may be useful to verify that the findings of based on PM performance measures can be extrapolated to daily functioning. Additionally, further examination of the structural and functional underpinnings of PM at this age may help reveal mechanisms of this possible cognitive resilience. References American Psychiatric Association. (1991). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author. Arana, J., Blanco, C., Meilan, J., Perez, E., Carro, J., & Gordillo, F. (2011). The impact of poly drug use on several prospective memory measures in a sample of university students. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 43, 109–131. Benoit, R. G., Gilbert, S. J., & Burgess, P. W. (2011). A neural mechanism mediating the impact of episodic prospection on farsighted decisions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 6771–6779. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6559-10.2011 Brandimonte, M., Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (Eds.). (1996). Prospective memory: Theory and applications. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Brown, S. A., Tapert, S. F., Granholm, E., & Delis, D. C. (2000). Neurocognitive functioning of adolescents: Effects of protracted alcohol use. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24, 164–171. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2000.tb04586.x Burgess, P. W., Alderman, N., Volle, E., Benoit, R. G., & Gilbert, S. J. (2009). Mesulam’s frontal lobe mystery re-examined. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 27, 493–506. Burgess, P. W., Gonen-Yaacovi, G., & Volle, E. (2011). Functional neuroimaging studies of prospective memory: What have we learnt so far?Neuropsychologia, 49, 2246–2257. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.014 Burgess, P. W., Quayle, A., & Frith, C. D. (2001). Brain regions involved in prospective memory as determined by positron emission tomography. Neuropsychologia, 39, 545–555. 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00149-4 Carey, C. L., Woods, S. P., Rippeth, J. D., Heaton, R. K., & Grant, I., & the HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center (HNRC) Group. (2006). Prospective memory in HIV-1 infection. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 536–548. 10.1080/13803390590949494 Casey, B. J., & Jones, R. M. (2010). Neurobiolgoy of the adolescent brain and behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 1189–1285. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2010). Binge drinking. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/alcohol/fact-sheets/binge-drinking.htm Chen, C. M., Dufour, M. C., & Yi, H. Y. (2004). Alcohol consumption among young adults ages 18–24 in the United States: Results from the 2001–02 NESARC survey. Alcohol Research & Health, 28, 269–280. Crews, F., He, J., & Hodge, C. (2007). Adolescent cortical development: A critical period of vulnerability for addiction. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 86, 189–199. 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.12.001 Dager, A. D., Anderson, B. M., Stevens, M. C., Pulido, C., Rosen, R., Jiantonio-Kelly, R. E., . . .Pearlson, G. D. (2013). Influence of alcohol use and family history of alcoholism on neural response to alcohol cues in college drinkers. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 37 (Suppl. 1), E161–E171. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2012.01879.x Diamond, I., & Jay, C. A. (2000). Alcoholism and alcohol use. In L.Goldman & J. C.Bennett (Eds.), Cecil textbook of medicine (21st ed., pp. 49–54). Philadelphia, PA: W. B. Saunders Company. Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 717–726. 10.1037/0278-7393.16.4.717 Glisky, E. (1996). Prospective memory and the frontal lobes. In M.Brandimonte, G.Einstein, & M.McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 327–350). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Gonneaud, J., Rauchs, G., Groussard, M., Landeau, B., Mézenge, F., de La Sayette, V., . . .Desgranges, B. (2014). How do we process event-based and time-based intentions in the brain? an fMRI study of prospective memory in healthy individuals. Human Brain Mapping, 35, 3066–3082. 10.1002/hbm.22385 Griffiths, A., Hill, R., Morgan, C., Rendell, P. G., Karimi, K., Wanagaratne, S., & Curran, H. V. (2012). Prospective memory and future event simulation in individuals with alcohol dependence. Addiction, 107, 1809–1816. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03941.x Harris, J., & Wilkens, A. (1982). Remembering to do things: A theoretical framework and illustrative experiment. Human Learning, 1, 123–136. Heffernan, T. M., & Bartholomew, J. (2006). Does excessive alcohol use in teenagers affect their everyday prospective memory?Journal of Adolescent Health, 39, 138–140. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2005.10.010 Heffernan, T. M., O’Neill, T., Ling, J., Holroyd, S., Bartholomew, J., & Betney, G. (2006). Does excessive alcohol use in teenagers affect their everyday prospective memory?Clinical Effectiveness in Nursing, eS39, e302–e307. Heffernan, T., Clark, R., Bartholomew, J., Ling, J., & Stephens, R. (2010). Does binge drinking in teenagers affect their everyday prospective memory?Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 109, 73–78. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.12.013 Heffernan, T., & O’Neill, T. (2012). Time based prospective memory deficits associated with binge drinking: Evidence from the Cambridge Prospective Memory Test (CAMPROMPT). Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 123, 207–212. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.11.014 Jacobus, J., & Tapert, S. F. (2013). Neurotoxic effects of alcohol in adolescence. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 703–721. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185610 Kahler, C. W., Hustad, J., Barnett, N. P., Strong, D. R., & Borsari, B. (2008). Validation of the 30-day version of the Brief Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire for use in longitudinal studies. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 69, 611–615. 10.15288/jsad.2008.69.611 Kvavilashvili, L. (1992). Remembering intentions: A critical review of existing experimental paradigms. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 6, 507–524. 10.1002/acp.2350060605 Kvavilashvili, L., & Ellis, J. (1996). Let’s forget the everyday/laboratory controversy. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 199–200. 10.1017/S0140525X00042254 Levy, R., & Loftus, G. (1984). Compliance and memory. In J.Harris & P.Morris (Eds.), Everyday memory, actions, and absent-mindedness (pp. 93–112). London, UK: Academic Press. Ling, J., Heffernan, T. M., Buchanan, T., Rodgers, J., Scholey, A. B., & Parrott, A. C. (2003). Effects of alcohol on subjective ratings of prospective and everyday memory deficits. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 27, 970–974. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2003.tb04422.x McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2000). Strategic and automatic processes in prospective remembering: A multiprocess framework. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, 127–144. Medina, K. L., McQueeny, T., Nagel, B. J., Hanson, K. L., Schweinsburg, A. D., & Tapert, S. F. (2008). Prefrontal cortex volumes in adolescents with alcohol use disorders: Unique gender effects. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 32, 386–394. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00602.x National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. (2004). NIAAA council approves definition of binge drinking. NIAAA Newsletter. Okuda, J., Fujii, T., Ohtake, H., Tsukiura, T., Yamadori, A., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2007). Differential involvement of regions of rostral prefrontal cortex (Brodmann area 10) in time- and event-based prospective memory. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 64, 233–246. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.09.009 Okuda, J., Fujii, T., Yamadori, A., Kawashima, R., Tsukiura, T., Fukatsu, R., . . .Fukuda, H. (1998). Participation of the prefrontal cortices in prospective memory: Evidence from a PET study in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 253, 127–130. 10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00628-4 Park, D. C., Hertzog, C., Kidder, D. P., Morrell, R. W., & Mayhorn, C. B. (1997). Effect of age on event-based and time-based prospective memory. Psychology and Aging, 12, 314–327. 10.1037/0882-7974.12.2.314 Pierce, R. C., & Kumaresan, V. (2006). The mesolimbic dopamine system: The final common pathway for the reinforcing effect of drugs of abuse?Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 215–238. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.016 Platt, B., Kamboj, S. K., Italiano, T., Rendell, P. G., & Curran, H. V. (2016). Prospective memory impairments in heavy social drinkers are partially overcome by future event simulation. Psychopharmacology, 233, 499–506. 10.1007/s00213-015-4145-1 Poppenk, J., Moscovitch, M., McIntosh, A. R., Ozcelik, E., & Craik, F. I. (2010). Encoding the future: Successful processing of intentions engages predictive brain networks. NeuroImage, 49, 905–913. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.049 Raskin, S. A. (2009). Memory for Intentions Screening Test: Psychometric properties and clinical evidence. Brain Impairment, 10, 23–33. 10.1375/brim.10.1.23 Raskin, S. A., Buckheit, C. A., & Waxman, A. (2012). Effect of type of cue, type of response, time delay and two different ongoing tasks on prospective memory functioning after acquired brain injury. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 22, 40–64. 10.1080/09602011.2011.632908 Raskin, S. A., Maye, J., Rogers, A., Correll, D., Zamroziewicz, M., & Kurtz, M. (2014). Prospective memory in schizophrenia: Relationship to medication management skills, neurocognition, and symptoms in individuals with schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 28, 359–365. 10.1037/neu0000040 Raskin, S. A., Woods, S. P., Poquette, A. J., McTaggart, A. B., Sethna, J., Williams, R. C., & Tröster, A. I. (2011). A differential deficit in time- versus event-based prospective memory in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology, 25, 201–209. 10.1037/a0020999 Rohsenow, D. J. (1983). Drinking habits and expectancies about alcohol’s effects for self versus others. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 752–756. 10.1037/0022-006X.51.5.752 Rose, M. E., & Grant, J. E. (2010). Alcohol-induced blackout. Phenomenology, biological basis, and gender differences. Journal of Addiction Medicine, 4, 61–73. 10.1097/ADM.0b013e3181e1299d Salthouse, T. A., Berish, D. E., & Siedlecki, K. L. (2004). Construct validity and age sensitivity of prospective memory. Memory & Cognition, 32, 1133–1148. 10.3758/BF03196887 Sanhueza, C., García-Moreno, L. M., & Expósito, J. (2011). Weekend alcoholism in youth and neurocognitive aging. Psicothema, 23, 209–214. Schuckit, M. A., Tiff, J. E., Smith, T. L., Wies-Beck, G. A., & Kalmtin, J. (1997). The relationship between self-rating of the effects (SRE) of alcohol and alcohol challenge results in ninety-eight young men. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 58, 397–404. 10.15288/jsa.1997.58.397 Schweinsburg, A. D., McQueeny, T., Nagel, B. J., Eyler, L. T., & Tapert, S. F. (2010). A preliminary study of functional magnetic resonance imaging response during verbal encoding among adolescent binge drinkers. Alcohol, 44, 111–117. 10.1016/j.alcohol.2009.09.032 Sellen, A. J., Louie, G., Harris, J. E., & Wilkins, A. J. (1997). What brings intentions to mind? An in situ study of prospective memory. Memory, 5, 483–507. 10.1080/741941433 Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., . . .Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M. I. N. I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM–IV and ICD-10. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59 (Suppl. 20), 22–33. Simons, J. S., Schölvinck, M. L., Gilbert, S. J., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2006). Differential components of prospective memory? Evidence from fMRI. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1388–1397. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.005 Smith, R. E., & Bayen, U. J. (2004). A multinomial model of event-based prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 756–777. 10.1037/0278-7393.30.4.756 Sobell, L. C., Maisto, S. A., Sobell, M. B., & Cooper, A. M. (1979). Reliability of alcohol abusers’ self-reports of drinking behavior. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 17, 157–160. 10.1016/0005-7967(79)90025-1 Sowell, E. R., Delis, D., Stiles, J., & Jernigan, T. L. (2001). Improved memory functioning and frontal lobe maturation between childhood and adolescence: A structural MRI study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7, 312–322. 10.1017/S135561770173305X Thomasius, R., Petersen, K., Buchert, R., Andresen, B., Zapletalova, P., Wartberg, L., . . .Schmoldt, A. (2003). Mood, cognition and serotonin transporter availability in current and former ecstasy (MDMA) users. Psychopharmacology, 167, 85–96. Umeda, S., Nagumo, Y., & Kato, M. (2006). Dissociative contributions of medial temporal and frontal regions to prospective remembering. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 17, 267–278. 10.1515/REVNEURO.2006.17.1-2.267 Wang, L., Altgassen, M., Liu, W., Xiong, W., Akgün, C., & Kliegel, M. (2011). Prospective memory across adolescence: The effects of age and cue focality. Developmental Psychology, 47, 226–232. 10.1037/a0021306 Wang, L., Kliegel, M., Yang, Z., & Liu, W. (2006). Prospective memory performance across adolescence. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 167, 179–188. 10.3200/GNTP.167.2.179-188 Ward, H., Shum, D., McKinlay, L., Baker-Tweney, S., & Wallace, G. (2005). Development of prospective memory: Tasks based on the prefrontal-lobe model. Child Neuropsychology, 11, 527–549. 10.1080/09297040490920186 Weinborn, M., Moyle, J., Bucks, R. S., Stritzke, W., Leighton, A., & Woods, S. P. (2013). Time-based prospective memory predicts engagement in risk behaviors among substance users: Results from clinical and nonclinical samples. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19, 284–294. 10.1017/S1355617712001361 Weinborn, M., Woods, S. P., O’Toole, S., Kellogg, E. J., & Moyle, J. (2011). Prospective memory in substance abusers at treatment entry: Associations with education, neuropsychological functioning, and everyday memory lapses. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 26, 746–755. 10.1093/arclin/acr071 Weissenborn, R., & Duka, T. (2003). Acute alcohol effects on cognitive function in social drinkers: Their relationship to drinking habits. Psychopharmacology, 165, 306–312. Welch, K. A., Carson, A., & Lawrie, S. M. (2013). Brain structure in adolescents and young adults with alcohol problems: Systematic review of imaging studies. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 48, 433–444. 10.1093/alcalc/agt037 West, R. (2008). The cognitive neuroscience of prospective memory. In M.Kliegel, M. A.McDaniel, & G. O.Einstein (Eds.), Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, development, and applied perspectives (pp. 261–282). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis Group/Erlbaum. White, A. M., Signer, M. L., Kraus, C. L., & Swartzwelder, H. S. (2004). Experiential aspects of alcohol-induced blackouts among college students. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 30, 205–224. 10.1081/ADA-120029874 Wilkens, A., & Baddeley, A. (1988). Remembering to recall in everyday life: An approach to absent mindedness. In M.Gruneberg, P.Morris, & R.Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues (Vol. 1). London, UK: Wiley and Sons. Wilson, B. (1987). The rehabilitation of memory. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Winward, J. L., Hanson, K. L., Bekman, N. M., Tapert, S. F., & Brown, S. A. (2014). Adolescent heavy episodic drinking: Neurocognitive functioning during early abstinence. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20, 218–229. 10.1017/S1355617713001410 Woods, S. P., Moran, L. M., Carey, C. L., Dawson, M. S., Iudicello, J. E., Gibson, S., . . . the HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center Group. (2008). Prospective memory in HIV infection: Is “remembering to remember” a unique predictor of self-reported medication management?Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, 257–270. 10.1016/j.acn.2007.12.006 Ziegler, T., Schultz-Darken, N., Scott, J., Snowdon, C., & Ferris, C. (2005). Neuroendocrine response to female ovulatory odors depends upon social condition in male common marmosets, Callithrix jacchus. Hormones and Behavior, 47, 56–64. Zöllig, J., West, R., Martin, M., Altgassen, M., Lemke, U., & Kliegel, M. (2007). Neural correlates of prospective memory across the lifespan. Neuropsychologia, 45, 3299–3314. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.06.010 Submitted: February 21, 2014 Revised: July 27, 2016 Accepted: July 31, 2016 This publication is protected by US and international copyright laws and its content may not be copied without the copyright holders express written permission except for the print or download capabilities of the retrieval software used for access. This content is intended solely for the use of the individual user. Source: Neuropsychology. Vol. 31. (2), Feb, 2017 pp. 191-199) Accession Number: 2016-55069-001 Digital Object Identifier: 10.1037/neu0000313

Let Us write for you! We offer custom paper writing services Order Now.

REVIEWS


Criminology Order #: 564575

“ This is exactly what I needed . Thank you so much.”

Joanna David.


Communications and Media Order #: 564566
"Great job, completed quicker than expected. Thank you very much!"

Peggy Smith.

Art Order #: 563708
Thanks a million to the great team.

Harrison James.


"Very efficient definitely recommend this site for help getting your assignments to help"

Hannah Seven